четверг, 15 марта 2012 г.

Bush condemns bombing in Lebanon; tells Syria, Iran to stop interfering in Beirut politics

President George W. Bush on Saturday bemoaned the latest assassination of a Lebanese official and told Syria and Iran to stop meddling in its neighbor's affairs. Investigators tried to determine if the killing was tied to attacks against anti-Syrian politicians.

Iran, which supports the Syrian-based opposition in a power struggle with Beirut's Western-allied government, came under criticism along with Syria for working to undermine Lebanese institutions.

"We demand that Syria, Iran and their allies end their interference in and obstruction of Lebanon's political process," the president said in a statement.

Syria has been blamed for the …

Free IT training

SOUTHMEAD: Free computer training is available for people aged 19and over at Southmead Development Trust's …

Gotcha! Complying With Financial Regulations ; The process never ends Bringing your systems into compliance with a new financial reporting law may be your biggest information- technology expenditure this decade.

Did you know that:

The process never ends Bringing your systems into compliance with a new financial reporting law may be your biggest information- technology expenditure this decade. Some say implementing compliance with the Sarbanes-Oxley Act is a bigger project than Y2K bug fixes. "That might seem extreme," says Kraig Haber, director of financial product marketing at SAP. "But Y2K was a single task - this is a recurring one."

"We've seen clients invest tens of thousands of hours to achieve minimal Sarbanes-Oxley compliance," says Gregory Derderian, managing director of the World-Class Finance practice at consultancy BearingPoint.

Speed is a requisite …

среда, 14 марта 2012 г.

Up, up and away in lawn chair hoisted by 150 helium balloons _ Oregon man heads to Idaho

A man has taken flight in a lawn chair hoisted by more than 150 large helium-filled party balloons in a bid to ride the wind from the central Oregon town of Bend all the way to Idaho.

Kent Couch was wearing a parachute Saturday morning as he kissed his wife and kids goodbye, patted …

Microsoft hopes for a fresh start with Windows 7

Microsoft Corp. put a new edition of Windows on sale Thursday, hoping for a fresh start after a bad reception for the previous version of the software that runs most of the world's personal computers.

Windows 7 is now available on new computers, and as a software upgrade for some older PCs.

A Fry's Electronics store in Renton, Wash., several miles south of Microsoft's headquarters in Redmond, opened at midnight to give customers an early shot at buying a new PC or a disc that they could use to put Windows 7 on their existing computers. Such upgrade discs start at $120.

"We're geeks, that's what geeks do. This is …

Dow's '97 gain wiped out

NEW YORK The Dow Jones industrial average tumbled 148.36 pointsFriday as the stock market resumed a downward spiral amid signs ofhigher inflation.

The Dow fell to 6,391.69. It was the third drop of more than140 points in 11 sessions. The average, which a month ago was 10percent ahead for the year, is now down almost 1 percent for 1997.

Other market indicators also posted stiff losses: The Standard &Poor's 500 stock list fell 20.69 to 737.65, slightly below …

Spanish court backs YouTube in copyright case

MADRID (AP) — A Spanish court has dismissed charges of copyright infringement brought against YouTube by television company Telecinco.

Telecinco said YouTube permitted material it owned to be uploaded and aired on the popular Internet video site, infringing its intellectual property rights.

The Madrid mercantile court rejected the claim. It said in a ruling Thursday that YouTube offers content owners …

NKorea sentences US man to 8 years of hard labor

North Korea has sentenced an American teacher to eight years of hard labor and ordered him to pay a $700,000 fine after he crossed illegally into the country _ the fourth U.S. citizen to be detained by the isolated regime since last year.

Aijalon Mahli Gomes, of Boston, acknowledged his wrongdoing during a trial at the Central Court on Tuesday, the North's official Korean Central News Agency said in a brief dispatch Wednesday.

The North said last month it arrested Gomes, 30, on Jan. 25 for trespassing after he crossed into the country from China.

Gomes, a graduate of Bowdoin College in Maine, had been teaching English in South Korea and no …

The Phantom's Transforming Experience

A Broadway leading man's face is his fortune -- except,perhaps, when it comes to "The Phantom of the Opera." In taking ontheater's most successful show, John Cudia, whose long turn as Raoulearned him a cadre of star-struck fans, had to come to grips withplaying not beauty but the beast this time around.

"Obviously, it's given my career tremendous exposure, but not meexactly," Cudia says with wry good humor. "It's so high-powered, butit's also anonymous."

And uncomfortable. The Phantom's iconic white domino is only theoutermost of several layers of disguise -- pancake, powder, paint,latex and human hair. Cudia, 36, who stars in the current productionat the …

Garnett has 22 points, 20 rebounds, leads Boston to opening 103-83 win over Washington

Kevin Garnett had 22 points, 20 rebounds and five assists, Paul Pierce scored 28 points and sparked Boston's take-charge rally in the first half, and the Celtics opened their promising season with a 103-83 victory over the Washington Wizards on Friday night.

Add 17 points from Ray Allen, another veteran newcomer, and the Celtics looked nothing like the teams Pierce played for in his first nine seasons with Boston. Those teams won just three playoff series.

Garnett, Pierce, and Allen left the game to a standing ovation with 2:30 left and the score 98-75. Garnett and Pierce raised their right index fingers to the screaming crowd, then Garnett knelt in front …

Couillard, Hon. Philippe, M.D. (Mont-Royal) Minister of Health and Social Services

COUILLARD, HON. PHILIPPE, M.D. (Mont-Royal) Minister of Health and Social Services.

B. Jun. 27, 1957 in Montreal. Ed. at Univ. of Montreal. M. to Suzanne Pilote. Five children. Political Career: First elected to the Que. Nat. Assembly e.g. 2003. Chair: Ctee ministerial du developpement social. Private Career: Head surgeon. Party: P.L.Q. Address: Leg. Office: �difice Catherine-de-Longpr�, 1075, chemin Sainte-Foy, 15e �tage, Qu�bec, Qu�., G1S 2M1, (418)266-7171, Fax: (418)266-7197; Riding Office: 3400, rue Jean-Talon Ouest, Bureau 100, Montreal, Qu�., H3R 2E8 (514)341-1151, Fax: (514)341-4777; Email: ministre@msss.gouv.qc.ca.


COUILLARD, HON. …

Nomo 1-Hitter Impresses Giants

Los Angeles Dodgers ace Hideo Nomo didn't even have his beststuff when he pitched a 3-0 one-hitter Saturday against the SanFrancisco Giants. And the Giants say it shows how well Nomo knowsthe game.

"I've seen him have better command as far as his splitter," saidGiants shortstop Royce Clayton, who broke up Nomo's no-hit bid with asingle to deep short with two outs in the seventh inning. "He had alot of giddy-up on his fastball and seemed to throw that well. Hehad enough control and velocity on his fastball to get guys out, andthat's what he did."

Nomo (9-2) lowered his ERA to 1.89 and struck out 11 to increasehis league-leading total to 161. He has four complete games andthree shutouts in 18 big-league starts.

Afterward, Dodgers manager Tommy Lasorda was almost nonchalantin talking about Nomo.

"He's pitched a lot of them like that," Lasorda said. "I thinkhe's pitched this well before. He's been that consistent all year."

As for Nomo, his pitching seemed less memorable than hisbatting. He outhit the Giants 2-1 and recorded his firstmajor-league RBI with a single in the ninth.

MANTLE SETBACK: Former New York Yankees great Mickey Mantlereceived another blood transfusion Sunday, and his doctor said hecould face more of them in his fight against cancer.

Mantle, 63, is suffering from anemia brought on by chemotherapy used to treat his lung cancer. The cancer spreadfrom his liver, which was so diseased that he underwent a transplantJune 8.

"It may not be that unusual if 48 hours from now we have to do(another transfusion)," said Dr. Daniel DeMarco, Mantle'sgastroenterologist. "He's relaxing and watching TV. His spirits arereally pretty good."

Mantle received his first transfusion Friday, but DeMarco saidtests taken early Sunday showed his blood count wasn't as high as itshould be.

Mantle remains in stable condition, but the extra treatmentmeans he won't be going home today as planned. DeMarco said Mantlelikely won't be released for at least a few more days.

BREWERS ADD DIBBLE: The Milwaukee Brewers added former White Soxreliever Rob Dibble to their 25-man big-league roster.

Dibble, 31, was signed by the Brewers to a minor-league contractJuly 29. He appeared in four games with Class AAA New Orleans anddidn't allow an earned run. Dibble was released by the Sox on July16 after going 0-1 with a 6.28 ERA.

Ironically, Dibble can't appear in a game until Wednesdaybecause of his part in a July 15 fight with the Brewers as a memberof the Sox.

EX-CUBS PLAYER DIES: Chicago native Dick Bartell, a shortstopwho played in three World Series and the first All-Star Game, is deadat 87.

Bartell died late Friday at a convalescent hospital in Alameda,Calif., after a battle with Alzheimer's disease. He compiled alifetime .284 average from 1927 to 1946 and played for the Cubs in1939. He hit .238 with three home runs and 34 RBI that season.

A NEW KIND OF ENGINEER

THE PHARMACEUTICAL INDUSTRY NEEDED SOMEONE WHO COULD MANAGE THE EXPENSIVE TASK OF DEVELOPING DRUGS. ENTER THE PHARMACEUTICAL ENGINEER

Over the course of the 20th century, the average American's life expectancy went from 47 in 1900 to 77 in 2000. Although a number of factors led to this unprecedented jump, much of the credit belongs to the development of modem medicine. New medicines don't just save and extend lives. They also improve the quality of it, with treatments for everything from asthma and depression to acne and impotence.

But this cornucopia of good health comes at a cost, and a hefty one at that. In 2003 the Tufts Center for the Study of Drug Development pegged the cost of developing a new prescription drug at nearly $900 million. This puts the pharmaceutical industry in an unenviable position in America's healthcare debate. On the one hand, the aging population's increased pharmaceutical use creates greater demand for new medications while pressure to reduce the cost of those medicines increases as well.

To solve these problems the industry looks to a new breed of engineer-the pharmaceutical engineer-to streamline the complex scientific, business, and regulatory processes of bringing a drug to market. The term "pharmaceutical engineering" has a certain appeal, promising lucrative and fulfilling work within the pharmaceutical industry. But ask five different pharmaceutical engineers to define the field and you'll get five different definitions. Some say it applies to the late stages of the drug-making process, and others to issues-like lab design, air handling, and water purification-surrounding the running of pharmaceutical production facilities.

Most engineering educators take a broader perspective on the field. Fernando Muzzio, director of Rutgers University's pharmaceutical engineering program and a professor of chemical and biochemical engineering at the school, says that he sees pharmaceutical engineers' responsibility as the rigorous and systematic use of engineering concepts and methods to scientifically design, develop, and optimize pharmaceutical processes and products. Pharmaceutical engineers know pharmaceutical materials and how products and industry are related. They can also work alongside chemists, biologists, pharmacologists, and regulators.

Henry Wang, director of the pharmaceutical engineering program at the University of Michigan, sees the field in even broader terms. Not only does he consider the development of traditional medicines to be within pharmaceutical engineering's domain but also biomedical devices, biologies like vaccines and gene therapy, and diagnostics. He also cites the emerging 'omics' fields like genomics, proteomics, and personalized medicine as future avenues of pharmaceutical engineering pursuit. "I originally wanted to call it Healthcare Product Engineering," Wang says about the program he helped to start at Michigan, "but that didn't resonate with students and faculty."

But how can a field that's so nebulously defined systematize the complicated drug discovery process? Engineering educators say that because a pharmaceutical engineer has such a broad understanding of process, he or she has a better view of the big picture. "Pharmaceutical engineering really helps people step back and see the industry as a whole," says David Wild, an adjunct professor in Michigan's pharmaceutical engineering program and director of his own scientific computing consulting company, Wild Ideas Consulting.

"There are all these different stages in the drug discovery process, and each stage is dominated by a particular discipline," Wild explains. Historically, the people working in each of these stages-chemistry, biology, pharmacology, engineering, and manufacturing-have tended to do so without necessarily considering the work in subsequent stages. Because of this, scientists spend much time and money working on drug candidates that ultimately fail. According to the Pharmaceutical Research and Manufacturers of America, for every 5,000 potential medicines tested, only five will make it into clinical trials. Of those five, only one will be approved for patient use. In fact, experts attribute as much as 75 percent of the total cost of each marketed drug to the high failure rates of other drug candidates.

Quite often a candidate from one stage in the drug discovery process fails for reasons, such as toxicity or expense of manufacturing, that another stage might have predicted. The intensive clinical trial and FDA approval process-along with complex regulatory, business, and legal issues-can also slow a promising drug candidate's progress to market. And since a new drug makes its developer the majority of its money under patent protection, every delay is costly. Ideally, a pharmaceutical engineer can make the entire process faster, more efficient, and therefore more profitable. "I think the best way of looking at it is that it's all the processes of the pharmaceutical industry viewed from an engineering prospective," Wild says.

Of course, training engineers with such a breadth of knowledge in just one particular industry requires a complex, multifaceted approach. While most educators see interdisciplinary study, cooperation with industry, and knowledge of FDA regulations as essential components in training to become a pharmaceutical engineer, they have different ideas about pharmaceutical engineering education.

PROXIMITY TO PHARMACEUTICALS

Over the past decade, a handful of pharmaceutical engineering programs have popped up across the country. Not surprisingly, most of the universities that have pharmaceutical engineering also have close ties, both geographically and financially, to major drug-making corporations. New Jersey-home to the headquarters of more global pharmaceutical and medical technology companies than any other state or country, for that matter, in the world-boasts pharmaceutical engineering programs at Rutgers University, New Jersey Institute of Technology, and Stevens Institute of Technology. Michigan's Wang says his university's proximity to what was Warner Lambert and is now Pfizer's Ann Arbor facility certainly helped in developing that school's program.

"I think the programs are responding to a perceived need by the pharmaceutical industry to bring in a type of individual who is well-versed in basic engineering principles, but also knowledgeable of regulatory aspects, manufacturing operations found in the pharmaceutical industry and who can communicate with life scientists carrying out the basic research as well as other engineering, architectural, and pharmacy graduates," says Henrik Pedersen, professor in Rutgers pharmaceutical engineering program and chair of the university's chemical and biochemical engineering department.

Although only a decade old, the Rutgers program is one of the oldest in the country. Program director Muzzio credits Carlos Rosas-who, at the time, was Merck's vice president of manufacturing-for conceiving what has become Rutgers' pharmaceutical engineering curriculum. Muzzio says that back in 1994 Rosas took him under his wing and told him, then an assistant professor, that the industry could really use engineers who understood the entire drug development and manufacturing process. So Muzzio began an interdisciplinary program that would give Rutgers' students the option of that specialization. Merck even provided some $50,000 in startup money.

"I used the funding to get the attention of my colleagues," Muzzio says, "and we were able to hire a number of young and eager assistant professors." Since there were basically no programs training potential young faculty in pharmaceutical engineering at the time, the new faculty were sent to do internships in industry.

Even now, professors continue to go back and work with pharmaceutical companies so they can stay abreast of the latest technologies in the field. Muzzio himself recently did an internship with Bristol-Myers Squibb in pharmaceutical operations and statistical design of pharmaceutical operations for a course he plans to teach. "I am learning as much as I am teaching," he says.

The Rutgers program is primarily research driven. Most students are working toward their Ph.D.'s. Undergraduates can take half a dozen courses in pharmaceutical engineering and can do research with professors in the program. Often the graduate students spend some time doing an internship with a pharmaceutical company and then do their doctoral research on a project funded by that company. "This is very much hands-on work," Muzzio explains. "They are immersed in the culture of collaboration."

Unlike those in other pharmaceutical engineering programs, students from Rutgers don't graduate with a pharmaceutical engineering degree. Instead they specialize in pharmaceutical engineering. That specialization complements a degree in a more traditional field like chemical or mechanical engineering. Muzzio takes responsibility for not having a pharmaceutical engineering degree. "We wanted to serve the students by giving them the most flexible degree."

There are currently 15 faculty members in the Rutgers program. Although chemical engineering serves as the program's home base, materials science, mechanical engineering, industrial engineering, and the school of pharmacy are all represented. "Chemical engineering provides a natural foundation for the field of pharmaceutical engineering," Pedersen says. "New methods better able to describe pharmaceutical operations and provide a rational and robust basis for regulatory guidelines are what have driven and will continue to drive the pharmaceutical engineering field to develop its own identity."

At the University of Michigan, the college of engineering and the college of pharmacy jointly administer the pharmaceutical engineering program. Since the program's inception in 2000, 16 students have earned master's degrees in pharmaceutical engineering and a doctoral training program was established last year.

"We want the students to know both the product quality and process efficiency," says Wang, director of Michigan's pharmaceutical engineering program and a professor of chemical and biomedical engineering. So that students get a comprehensive view of the drug development and discovery process, Michigan brings in adjunct faculty from industry and government to teach alongside the school's science and engineering faculty.

The curriculum includes a core course in pharmaceutical engineering that Wang teaches. "Basically, I introduce the students to the industry, what are the engineering issues in drug discovery, and manufacturing," he says. There are also seminar courses with speakers from industry, the FDA, and academia. To get the broad, yet specialized base in pharmaceutical engineering, students also usually take advanced science and advanced engineering statistics, analytical chemistry, and a course in intellectual property from the business school. Most students opt to take a regulatory science course as well, although Wang says in the near future this course will be a requirement for all students in the program.

Research and practical training are also important components of the program. About a third of the students arc pursuing the degree part time while holding down jobs in the industry. Those who don't already have industrial jobs often choose to include internships or co-ops as part of their studies. Wang stresses that whatever project the students work on during these internships, they must be relevant to their training as pharmaceutical engineers. To that end, the students have both an industrial as well as academic adviser.

Jennifer VanRoeyen got her master's from the Michigan program in 2003 and currently works with ZS Associates, a consulting firm specializing in the healthcare industry. She has done two different internships, one with Eli Lilly and Merck on process development and one with Janssen Pharmaceutica, a Belgian subsidiary of Johnson & Johnson, on product development. VanRoeyen says that both experiences gave her an appreciation of all the opportunities the pharmaceutical engineering degree could provide. "As an engineer," she says, "it was a wonderful way to figure where I could fit in at a pharmaceutical company."

Another way that engineers gain specialization in pharmaceutical engineering is through continuing education courses. Michael F. Waxman is one of 30 professors who do outreach teaching in engineering through the University of Wisconsin. Students in the program get continuing education credit from the reputable University of Wisconsin without having to actually go to Wisconsin-which depending upon the season and your interests can be considerably less attractive than where the courses are usually taught. Las Vegas, for instance, is one popular location.

The university offers as many as 30 different short courses in highly specialized aspects of pharmaceutical engineering. Waxman says for two or three days he'll teach an in-depth course on some facet of pharmaceutical engineering to a class of 20 to 150 students, depending upon the subject. Waxman says his students are chemists, biologists, and engineers that work for pharmaceutical engineering companies around the world.

Waxman says continuing education courses like his are attractive to the pharmaceutical industry because they give engineers specific knowledge they need and generally do not learn in school. And because people working in the industry teach the courses, students also keep up to date with the newest technologies and methods and the latest government regulations. "My students leave appreciating the education they get because they know what they need when they're working in industry," Waxman says. "They can use what they've learned the next day."

Educators from Rutgers and Michigan also say their students find the pharmaceutical engineering training to be highly rewarding. "We never have any problems placing our students. They are vigorously recruited by industry," Rutgers' Muzzio says. "I think this is going to be one of the most active growth areas for the profession."

Michigan's Wild echoes this sentiment: "The people who have pharmaceutical experience and the piece of paper to prove it are really going to have the advantage in terms of jobs."

[Sidebar]

THIS LITTLE PILL WENT TO MARKET

By the time a potential drug is taken by a human, it has spent about six-and-a-half years in the discovery/preclinical stage of drug development. During this stage, researchers perform laboratory and animal tests to determine how safe the drug is, how well it works to treat a particular disease, and what type of formulation will be most feasible for the particular compound.

Once completed, the company that made the drug must file an Investigational New Drug Application with the U.S. Food and Drug Administration (FDA) before the drug can advance to clinical trials. For every 5,000 compounds that go through the preclinical stage, only five will advance to human testing.

The human clinical trial phase of the drug development process occurs in three phases. In Phase I the drug is given to a group of 20 to 100 healthy volunteers to study the drug's safety. Researchers also learn how the drug works within the body: how it's absorbed, metabolized, and excreted, as well as how long it is effective. On average, it takes a year and a half to complete this phase.

During the two years of Phase II, clinical trials are conducted on 100 to 500 volunteers with the disease that the drug is being approved to treat. They will take the drug in order to evaluate its effectiveness and to see if there are any side effects.

Phase III clinical trials expand Phase II testing to between 1,000 and 5,000 patients in clinics and hospitals. At this stage, doctors monitor patients closely to confirm the efficacy observed in Phase II and to look for any adverse reactions from long-term use. Phase III can take up to three and a half years.

Once a drug successfully progresses through human clinical trials, its maker will compile all the data generated from the trials and submit it to the FDA in the form of a New Drug Application (NDA). The average NDA weighs in at 100,000 pages. The FDA review and approval process at this stage lasts about a year and a half. For every five drugs that enter human clinical trials only one will get the FDA's nod.

On average, the rigorous drug approval process lasts anywhere from 10 to 15 years, according to the Tufts Center for the Study of Drug Development. The center also estimates that it takes more than $800 million to get to this stage. But even after a drug goes on the market, its maker must periodically submit reports to the FDA. And some drugs must go through additional Phase IV trials to identify any adverse long-term effects. Aftermarket testing can add nearly $100 million in costs, bringing the final tab of drug discovery and development to about $900 million. Still, the hefty costs and high risks haven't kept drug companies from trying to bring new products onto the market. The Pharmaceutical Research and Manufacturers of America says that its member companies spent about $32.1 billion on research and development in 2002.

PERSONAL PHARMACEUTICALS

Ever wonder why a medication that works well on some might have absolutely no effect on others or might even make them violently ill? The answer, scientists say, is locked in our DNA.

Over the last few years, scientific advances like proteomics, DNA microarrays, and the sequencing of the human genome have given researchers insight into how DNA influences the diseases we get and how we respond to different medications. Scientists predict that because of this new genetic perspective on diseases and medications, we'll someday be able to walk into our doctor's office, take a simple genetic test, and get medicine that is tailor-made for us.

Pharmaceutical engineering educators agree that personalized medicine will play a big part in the future of their discipline. And although they say that their programs include faculty and courses in the "omics"-genomics, proteomics, pharmacogenomics-they caution that despite breathless media reports, personalized medicine is still very much in its nascent stages. "We can dream up all these things," says Fernando Muzzio, director of the pharmaceutical engineering program at Rutgers University, "but to make them real we are going to need the same basic engineering fields."

Muzzio adds that pharmaceutical engineers already have plenty of exciting challenges that can impact people's lives and healthcare. "It might not be the sexiest area of work, but it is the area where what we do could reach millions of people tomorrow," he says. "If we can improve the reliability of pediatric formulations, for example, then we can help kids right now. It doesn't matter if we're not being interviewed by Newsweek."

[Author Affiliation]

Bethany Halford is a freelance writer based in Baltimore.

вторник, 13 марта 2012 г.

Moss, Green lead TCU to 79-63 victory over Houston

FORT WORTH, Texas (AP) — Ronnie Moss scored 17 points and Garlon Green 16 and TCU pulled away in the second half for a 79-63 victory over Houston on Saturday.

Moss' effort was his ninth straight double-figure game dating to last season. J.R. Cadot scored 11 for the Horned Frogs (4-2), who shot 45.3 percent (29 of 64).

Adam Brown hit 4 of 6 3-pointers and led the Cougars (4-2) with 14 points. Kendrick Washington and Alandise Harris had 12 points each, and Zamal Nixon scored 10.

The Horned Frogs outrebounded Houston 36-31, led by Cadot's nine, and committed only 12 turnovers to the Cougars' 19.

Brown gave Houston its only lead on a 3-pointer with 9:11 left in the first half for a 21-20 edge.

TCU led 49-43 with 13:53 left before 7-0 spurt made it 56-43. The Horned Frogs coasted from there with a 43-32 second-half margin.

3 Man United fans attacked in Milan

Three Manchester United supporters were attacked by four Inter Milan fans in the aftermath of the Champions League match between the sides on Tuesday, ANSA news agency reports.

The incident occurred shortly after midnight, near the stadium, on a road connecting the San Siro with the nearest metro stop.

The United fans, aged 28, 48 and 49 were taken to San Carlo hospital in Milan for treatment. One suffered a broken nose and another a cut on the head. All three were later discharged.

The group of Inter fans escaped in a car before police could apprehend them. No motive was given for the attack.

SNOWBOARDING PIONEER; Despite celebrity status, Boise will always be home to this family man

He's been something of a wintertime Superman the past two decades, crisscrossing snow-covered hills the world over with a board strapped to his feet. Now he's content to simply play Super Dad.

And Super Dad is about to get some face time on the silver screen.

Boise's Nick Perata is a giant in the snowboarding industry, though you'd never know it by talking to the guy. In the mid-1980s, he was one of only a handful of people plunging down mountains on homemade boards and taking monthlong treks up into the mountains in order to find the best freeriding in the world.

He's now starred in eight movies about the sport and appeared on ABC and ESPN. He was running his own annual competition in Valdez, Alaska, when members of the film crew covering it from Transition Productions realized they were watching "the pinnacle of the sport," relates Perata, now 38.

Some four years later, Transition Productions asked him to participate in their action documentary about the history of snowboarding and a real-world, fraught-with-danger run down a middle-of-nowhere hill in Alaska by five of the biggest names in the sport.

He signed on, and the film is due out December 2. There's no word yet on an Idaho release date (though he's been assured it's definitely coming to Boise), but Perata is being flown out for a Hollywood-style premiere in New York City on December 1, and will attend peripheral publicity events, a meeting with film sponsor Pepsi's CEO and interviews on, among others programs, The Today Show.

First Descent, tagged as "The Story of the Snowboarding Revolution," covers the timeline "from breaking open barrels and standing on the wooden planks and going down the snow in the '30s and all that, to present day," Perata said.

It follows "what they were going through when they were making these boards in the '60s and '70s, all the way through me and my generation, which were really first-generation snowboarders."

The release of the film is exciting for Perata, though he admits his family and friends are more energized about tripping to the Big Apple.

In fact, for someone who spent the bulk of his young life as a snow bum, it's fascinating to note that the biggest highs he finds these days are spending time with his family.

No, he hasn't abandoned boarding--"When it's open and good, I'll go up [to Bogus Basin] two, three days a week"--but now his life is all about the wife and four kids.

He would love to see his children grow up to be professional snowboarders or golfers (golf is his other great passion) but for now, they aren't even allowed to board down a hill--that is, until they learn how to ski.

For the sake of learning all-important edge control, "I told them as soon as they can go anywhere on the hill on their skis then they can go with the full-time snowboard," Perata divulged.

When not working as a fishing guide in Alaska for four months over the summer, a typical day for Perata is dropping off his two eldest children (7 and 5 years old) at school, then returning home with his two youngest (2 years and 7 months old) to maintain the house and practice golf in the backyard. FYI, his 2-year-old has a killer swing.

He's traveled the world, snowboarding in Europe, New Zealand and South America and yet, he still calls Boise--and little ol' Bogus Basin Mountain Resort--home.

"Bogus is actually where I sharpened my skills enough to move on to bigger and better places ... Bogus has been the start for a lot of people--a lot of people who are still in the industry these days ... They will always come back to Boise. That's what I've noticed."

Perata has certainly always come back. From the time he first visited the city when he was only 13, he's loved it here, and he plans to stay for at least the next 20 years.

So what of Super Dad's resurgent celebrity status? Will he soon be too big for the valley?

"Celebrity? No, I'm just a dad, man," he said. "I love my kids. I'm my kids' hero."

And that is more than enough for this legend in the snowboarding world.

Article copyright Bar Bar Inc.

Photograph (A still from First Descent)

Local sports

MURRELL to carolina

Former West Virginia University running back Adrian Murrellagreed to terms on a one-year contract with the Carolina Panthers. Murrell, who rushed for 1,000 yards in 1996, 1997 and 1998, agreedto a one-year contract worth slightly less than $500,000, theCharlotte Observer reported.

The eighth-year veteran played last year in Washington and hasstint with the New York Jets and Arizona Cardinals. With theRedskins, he played 15 games but gained only a combined 143 yardsrushing and receiving.

He joins two other ex-Mountaineers, punter Todd Sauerbrun andoffensive lineman Tanner Russell, in Charlotte.

WVC FOOTBALL

Back-to-back road games at NCAA I-AA top-25 schools TennesseeTech and McNeese State highlight West Virginia University Tech's2001 football schedule. The West Virginia Conference on Wednesdayreleased schedules for its member schools.

Last year, WVU Tech played at I-AA Illinois State and lost 75-10. This coming season, the Golden Bears will travel to TennesseeTech on Sept. 22 and to McNeese State on Sept. 29. WVU Tech opensits season at 6 p.m. on Aug. 25 at Johnson C. Smith University inCharlotte, N.C. The Golden Bears have seven road games and four homegames.

West Virginia State opens its season Sept. 1 at home againstKentucky State. The Yellow Jackets play five home games, one ofwhich will be at Laidley Field, and six on the road, including aSept. 15 date at I-AA Charleston Southern.

State and Tech schedules are on this page.

WVC BASEBALL

West Virginia State is ranked second in the latest NCAA DivisionII North Atlantic Region rankings. Kutztown is No. 1. Also rankedare West Virginia Conference members Concord at No. 6 and WestVirginia Wesleyan and Shepherd, tied for No. 8.

BOOKER SELECTED

Riverside High School junior Karl Booker has been selected toplay in an all-star basketball tournament this summer in Hawaii.Booker was selected by Tourney Sport U.S.A. to participate in theJuly event, which showcases juniors from throughout the country.He'll spend 10 days in Hawaii.

WVU RUNNER HONORED

Distance runner Steve Bohan was named winner of the Red BrownCup.

That is awarded annually by West Virginia University totheschool's best all-around student-athlete.

A Toronto, Ontario, native, Bohan achieved All-America status forhis performance at the NCAA cross country championships this pastseason. He placed 18th, and became only the seventh Mountaineer tomake All-America in cross country.

Bohan, who won the Penn Relays 10,000 meters as a junior, is afive-time all-Big East honoree in cross country and track. He has a3.23 cumulative GPA in biology.

BASEBALL TRYOUT

The Charleston Patriots USSSA baseball team will conduct tryoutsfrom 3:30 to 5:30 p.m. May 13 at George Washington High School.Players must be 15-16 years old. For more information call 347-3238.

SENIOR SOFTBALL

Three area senior softball teams performed well in a recentsenior tournament in Myrtle Beach, S.C. The West Virginia Gold (ages50-54) lost in the championship game to Virginia Beach, 9-8. TobaccoExpress (60-64) won six games and finished as the runner-up. PackMart (55-59) finished fourth. A total of 33 teams were entered.

GOLF TOURNEY

Big Bend will host a blind-draw scramble on May 25. Entry fee is$35, which includes greens fees, cart and lunch.

MOUNTAINEER DINNER

The annual West Virginia University Mountaineer ScholarshipDinner is scheduled for 7 p.m. May 14 at the Charleston Marriott.Expected guests include Gov. Bob Wise, WVU Athletic Director EdPastilong and WVU football and basketball coaches Rick Rodriguez andGale Catlett. Tickets are $75. For more information call Don Gay at727-6403.

youth track

The regional meet for the Hershey's Track & Field Youth Programis scheduled for June 5 at 6 p.m. at Hurricane High School. The meetis for boys and girls ages 9-14. The state meet is scheduled forJune 30 at 6 p.m. at Cabell Midland High. For more information call562-0518.

herd sports

Marshall University's softball team opens a three-game serieswith Ohio today that could land it in the Mid-American Conferencetournament.

The Herd (34-21, 14-7 MAC) has won 15 of its last 18 games andsits in third place, just one game ahead of the Bobcats. Marshallmust win 2-of-3 from Ohio, or win one and get a Bowling Green sweepof Kent State to qualify.

Meanwhile, the Herd's baseball team (22-22, 9-11) plays theBobcats in a four-game series beginning today in Athens. MU thenhosts a doubleheader beginning at 1 p.m. Saturday.

softball clinics

The Huntington-based Diamond Dusters girls softball program willsponsor hitting and pitching clinics at the Huntington High softballfield. The hitting clinic is scheduled for May 21-22 and again May28-29 for girls ages 8-18. The pitching clinic is set for June 4-5and again June 11-12. For more information call Larry at 522-6083 orBill at 523-8127.

Holder raises question on Sept. 11 death penalty

Attorney General Eric Holder says there's a real question about whether a terrorist suspect such as alleged Sept. 11 mastermind Khalid Sheikh Mohammed can face the death penalty if he were to plead guilty before a military commission.

Holder proposed last year trying Mohammed and four alleged accomplices in civilian courts in New York City. But that idea generated so much controversy that it's all but been abandoned.

He told CBS' "Face the Nation" that it's possible to impose the death penalty in a civilian setting for someone who pleads guilty. But he says there's far less legal certainty about that possibility in a military setting.

Since January, Holder has said that all options are on the table about where to try Mohammed and the four other terrorist suspects. That includes the possibility of having them go before a military commission at the U.S. Navy base in Guantanamo Bay, Cuba, where they are now held.

Mohammed, who was captured in Pakistan in 2003, has proclaimed his involvement in the Sept. 11 plot and has said he wants to plead guilty and be executed, achieving what he views as martyrdom.

The attorney general said the Obama administration is working through issues about a site for the proceedings, taking into account the need for Congress to approve funding and trying to address concerns expressed by local officials.

"As soon as we can" resolve those issues, "we will make a decision as to where that trial will occur," Holder said.

He said "the politicization of this issue, when we're dealing with ultimate national security issues, is something that disturbs me a great deal."

The attorney general said it is his hope that Congress provides money to move Guantanamo detainees to a new location in Thomson, Illinois, where an underused state prison now exists.

"There is no reason to believe that people held in Guantanamo cannot be held wherever we put them in the United States. Again, very safely and very effectively," Holder said.

The need for congressional approval of the money for the project stands in the way of doing so, with Republicans and some Democrats objecting to bringing those prisoners into the United States.

Early-stage capital tight for entrepreneurs with ideas

REGION

Recessions often give rise to great business ideas, but many entrepreneurs are struggling to find investors who can help get those ideas off the ground.

At the Centre County-based seedcapital fund known as Ben Franklin Technology Partners, funding inquiries have doubled from ayear ago, said Pam Martin, director for the southcentral region.

"There's a lot more people in our pipeline, which means it's making competition for our limited funds even more competitive," she said.

The line outside Martin's door is growing because of tighter financing from banks and private early-stage investors, but also because many people try to start businesses when they lose their jobs, she said.

Meanwhile, companies in which Ben Franklin already has invested are finding it harder to move to the next stage of financing, so they are asking to stay under the organization's wing for longer, Martin said. That ties up more capital.

Ben Franklin is one of four nonprofits statewide that make investments with funding from the state government. Each got $6.9 million in the last fiscal year, but budget plans in Harrisburg would reduce the pot of money that funds the nonprofits, said Terry Singer, director of statewide affairs for the four groups.

Angel investors, individuals who make earlystage investments, are pulling back, Singer said.

"A lot of (angels) have lost a lot of money, just like everybody who was in the stock market and everybody who was in mutual funds," he said.

A little higher up the financing ladder, the venture capital outlook is bleak, according to a survey of the industry. Nationwide venture investments totaled $3 billion in the first quarter of 2009, down almost 50 percent from the previous quarter, according to the Moneytree report. It was the lowest level of venture investment since 1997, according to the report, compiled by the accounting firm PricewaterhouseCoopers and the National Venture Capital Association.

But several local examples paint a brighter picture.

The Lancaster Angel Network is still moving at the same pace, said Mike Shoemaker, one of its leaders. Some investors are saving cash in case firms in which they already have invested need it, but newcomers are still being funded, he said.

A Dauphin County company recently snagged a $1.1 million slice of the withered pie. Derry Township-based CoTweet Inc. has developed an eponymous application that helps businesses get their messages out on Twitter, the socialnetworking Web site.

"The reception overall was very, very positive because this is one area where people see growth," CoTweet Chief Executive Officer lesse Engle said.

The company announced the investment IuIy 9. At least one of the CoTweet investors, First Round Capital, has a Pennsylvania presence, with an office in suburban Philadelphia.

One of the keys to CoTweet's success was having advisers who knew the right people to call, Engle said. He found those advisers through personal networks developed over years in the technology industry, he said.

Lancaster County-based nanotechnology firm Illuminex Corp. is talking to potential investors but not searching aggressively for money, said CEO Joe Habib.

"Because we don't necessarily need it and we just feel that it's in our best interest to only talk to people that are serious and it's a very timeconsuming process," he said.

For now, the company is more focused on developing its technology than landing investors, Habib said.

"If you have a great technology, the money will come to you," he said.

So far, Illuminex has been funded by state and federal grants and some subcontracting, Habib said. Revenue is expected to reach more than $1 million in 2009.

The Investors Circle of York has been plugging along as usual through the downturn, said Darrell Auterson, president of the York County Economic Development Corp (YCEDC), which created the angel network. The last investment he could recall the group making was last fall.

The investors circle has always been rather conservative, Auterson said.

And he acknowledged that hunting for money And he acknowledged that hunting for money is no picnic.

"The challenge is that money is very tight in this market, and this recession is not your typical recession, given what happened in the capital markets," Auterson said. "I think it makes it that much more challenging to access capital."

[Sidebar]

"The reception overall was very, very positive because this is one area where people see growth."

Jesse Engle, CoTweet Inc.

[Sidebar]

YOUR TAKE

Have an opinion about this issue? E-mail us at editorial@journalpub.com.

[Author Affiliation]

BY DAVID DAGAN

davidd@journalpub.com

Cuban Defense Minister Julio Casas dies at 75

HAVANA (AP) — Cuban state television is reporting the death of Defense Minister Julio Casas. It says he suffered a heart attack at age 75, and officials have declared three days of mourning.

Casas fought in the revolution that brought Fidel Castro to power in 1959 and oversaw the military's lucrative economic enterprises for years before replacing Raul Castro as defense minister in early 2008.

State television said Casas died Saturday.

Behavioral phenotyping in zebrafish: Comparison of three behavioral quantification methods

The zebrafish has been popular in developmental biology and genetics, but its brain function has rarely been studied. High-throughput screening of mutation or drug-induced changes in brain function requires simple and automatable behavioral tests. This article compares three behavioral quantification methods in four simple behavioral paradigms that test a range of characteristics of adult zebrafish, including novelty-induced responses, social behavior, aggression, and predator-model-induced responses. Two quantification methods, manual recording and computerized videotracking of location and activity, yielded very similar results, suggesting that automated videotracking reliably measures activity parameters and will allow high-throughput screening. However, observation-based event recording of posture patterns was found generally not to correlate with videotracking measures, suggesting that further refinement of automated behavior quantification may be considered.

The zebrafish is a diploid vertebrate with a good balance of complexity and simplicity. It is small (4 cm long) and easy to keep and breed (Westerfield, 1993). Its aquarium environment is isolated from the experimenter's, which facilitates elimination of disturbing external stimuli. The zebrafish is prolific: A single female produces 200 eggs per spawning and can spawn several times a week. The fry grow quickly (reaching free-swimming stage within 5 days) and become sexually mature within 2 months (Delrich, Westerfield, & Zon, 1999). Large numbers of zebrafish can be kept in a small space, making the ease of housing comparable to that of invertebrate model organisms, such as C. elegans and Drosophila, and superior to that of other vertebrate species, such as the mouse or the rat. Coupled with these excellent features, its complex brain structure, similar in basic layout to other vertebrate brains, including our own (see Tropepe & Sive, 2003), and its sophisticated (albeit not yet well-utilized) behavior make the zebrafish an ideal model organism for neuroscience.

Several genes discovered in the zebrafish are evolutionarily conserved and have homologues in mammals, including our own species (e.g., Cerda, Conrad, Markl, Brand, & Herrmann, 1998). Syntenic relationships between regions of zebrafish and mammalian chromosomes are also known (Barbazuk et al., 2000; Woods et al., 2000). A large number of genetic tools allowing random or targeted introduction of mutations and the identification of the mutant genes are available. For example, genetic markers, linkage maps, and oligonucleotide microarrays aid localization and identification of randomly induced mutations (Donovan et al., 2000; Geisler et al., 1999; Guo et al., 2000; Hukriede et al., 1999; Knapik et al., 1998; Stickney et al., 2002; Zhang, Talbot, & Schier, 1998). Reverse genetic methods (McCallum, Comai, Griene, & Henikoff, 2000; Nasevicius & Ekker, 2000; Wienholds, Schultz-Merker, Walderich, & Plasterk, 2002) and the sequencing of the genome of zebrafish at the Sanger Center also favor this species, and most genetic tools and sequence information are in the public domain (e.g., Genbank [the Sanger Center Web site] and ZFIN; Sprague, Doerry, Douglas, & Westerfield, 2001). In sum, the genetics of the zebrafish place this species on par with the mouse or the fruit fly (Eisen, 1996; Granato & Nusslein-Volhard, 1996; Grunwald, 1996).

By now, hundreds of mutant zebrafish have been generated (see, e.g., Currie, 1996; Eisen, 1996; Grunwald, 1996; Haffter & Nusslein-Volhard, 1996; Holder & McMahon, 1996), but the majority of studies have focused on developmental questions (e.g., Ganger et al., 1998; Concha & Adams, 1998; Detrichet al., 1999; Eisen, 1991, 1996; Fetcho & O'Malley, 1995; Schier, 1997). Only a very few have attempted to investigate the genetics of behavior or brain function, and most of these studies have focused on the perceptual systems: the visual system (Baier et al., 1996; Brockerhoff et al., 1996; Neuhauss, 2003; Neuhauss et al., 1999), the olfactory system (e.g., Kratz, Dugas, & Ngai, 2002), the auditory system, and inner ear functions (Bang, Yelick, Malicki, & Sewell, 2002; Granato et al., 1996; Malicki et al., 1996). The motor function of the zebrafish has been studied in the embryo (e.g., Fetcho & Liu, 1998; Liu & Fetcho, 1999; Lorent, Liu, Fetcho, & Granato, 2001), as well as in the adult (Gerlai et al., 2000). Motor responses in the context of circadian activity patterns have been investigated in small fry (Zhdanova, Wang, Leclair, & Danilova, 2001). Furthermore, analyses of conditioned place preference (Darland & Dowling, 2001) and alcohol-dependent strain differences in social behavior (Dlugos & Rabin, 2003) suggest that the zebrafish has a sophisticated behavioral repertoire and that functional changes of the brain, induced by drugs of abuse, can be detected at the behavioral level. Strain differences in the development of the zebrafish, due to early ethanol exposure, have also been demonstrated recently (Loucks & Carvan, 2004), and behavioral effects of such exposure have been shown (Carvan, Loucks, Weber, & Williams, 2004).

The above demonstrates that behavioral studies using the zebrafish have started and that this species has promise in forward genetics and in behavioral neuroscience in general. This optimism was also reflected in a recent symposium "Zebrafish, a New Behavioral Model System" organized by one of us (see www.noldus.webaxxs.net/mb2005/program/index.html) at the Measuring Behavior 2005 conference (Wageningen, The Netherlands). The speakers at the symposium agreed that using this species as the subject of forward genetics or as a model system for drug screening and toxicology will have great utility in behavioral neuroscience. Before one can fully utilize the zebrafish, however, appropriate phenotypical test methods must be worked out. Particularly important is the development of a methodology that allows fast and reliable detection of functional changes in the brain in a manner that may be scaled up for high-throughput applications.

One fruitful way to detect functional alterations of the brain is to conduct a behavioral analysis (see, e.g., Gerlai & Clayton, 1999). Successful examples demonstrating the utility of behavior-analysis-based mutation screening abound in other species (see, e.g., Byers, Davis, & Kiger, 1981; Levin et al., 1992). The goal of the present article is to conduct a proof-of-concept analysis and show that behavioral paradigms and behavioral quantification methods useful for high-throughput screening are also feasible in the zebrafish.

Four simple paradigms developed previously (Gerlai, Lahav, Guo, & Rosenthal, 2000) are employed: a novel open tank, a social preference task, an aggression test, and a predator model test. These paradigms represent a variety of test conditions under which we previously detected idiosyncratic behavioral responses induced by alcohol (Gerlai et al., 2000). Here, the different test conditions will allow us to ask how three recording methods are capable of quantifying test-specific behavior-that is, environmentally induced changes in behavior. The three behavioral quantification methods compared are (1) manual recording of swim location and locomotor activity, a method employed before with the zebrafish; (2) videotracking-based analysis of swim paths that allows automation of recording and, thus, scaling up, a necessary requirement for high-throughput testing; and (3) computer-aided analysis (event recording) of motor and posture patterns, a method often utilized by ethologists.

METHOD

Animals and Housing

One hundred eighteen adult, 3- to 6-month-old male and female zebrafish (Danio rerio) were used. All the fish were purchased from a local vendor (Pet Pacifica, Honolulu) and were of a genetically heterogeneous (randomly bred) stock whose exact origin is not known. Most of the fish exhibited the long fin phenotype to a varying degree. The disadvantage of undefined genetic heterogeneity is that such a stock is difficult to use for forward genetic (e.g., random chemical-mutagenesis-based) studies (large genetic variability), but the advantage of this stock is hybrid vigor (e.g., ease of maintenance) and similarity to natural wild populations in terms of phenotypical features. Furthermore, genetic variability and, thus, increased phenotypical variance may allow one to better investigate phenotypical correlations, one of the goals of the present study.

The fish were kept in groups of 20 for 2 weeks in 40-1 quarantine tanks (50 � 30 � 26 cm, length � depth � width) and then moved to large 160-1 (90 � 60 � 30 cm) home tanks, where they were kept in groups of 80 until the experiments started. Thermostat-controlled heaters maintained the water temperature at 26� in all the tanks, and the water was filtered by Fluval 204 (small tanks) or Fluval 404 (large tanks) canister filters that contained filter foam (mechanical filtration), activated carbon (removal of organic waste and small particles), and BioMax rings (biological filtration). The fish tanks were illuminated using fluorescent light tubes (20 W/tank) switched on at 7:00 h and off at 19:00 h. The tanks also received natural light (sunrise around 6:00 h and sunset around 19:30 h). The fish were fed twice daily with a 50/50 mix of ground TetraMin flakes (Melle, Germany) and freeze-dried krill (Aquatic Ecosystems, Apopka, FL).

General Experimental Procedures

The behavior of the fish was recorded between 10:00 and 17:00 h in four test paradigms similar to those described previously (Gerlai et al., 2000). The fish were tested first in the novel open tank and then in the group preference paradigm, followed by an aggression (mirror) test and, finally, a predator model task. This constant order of the tests was established previously in order to minimize interference among tests and to minimize environmental error variation (Gerlai et al., 2000). In each test, the fish were placed individually into the experimental tank (20 � 25 � 12 cm, length � depth � width) and were monitored for 10 min. The intertest interval was 2 min. Upon conclusion of all four tests, the fish were returned to their home tank and were kept there for future experimentation. A CCD camera (Panasonic WV-CP470) fed the live image (frontal view) into the computer (Dell Dimension 8300, Pentium IV), and this image was processed using the EthoVision 3.0 video-tracking software (Noldus Information Technology, Wageningen, The Netherlands). A second camera (Sony DCR TRV 70) was used to record the frontal view of the experimental tank onto MiniDV tapes. The videorecordings on these tapes were later downloaded onto the computer and were analyzed using manual activity and location quantification, and also with The Noldus Observer Color Pro software.

Behavioral Tests

Novel open tank. Exposure to a novel test chamber, as well as handling by the experimenter, is an inherent part of most laboratory animal behavioral tests. The novel open tank task is intended to analyze behavior in response to these factors. In this task, zebrafish may exhibit elevated activity that habituates with time (Gerlai, 2003), and they may also show fear-related behaviors (Gerlai et al., 2000). These behavioral responses have previously been investigated with crude manual recording of locomotory activity and swim location. In addition to this method, both computerized event recording of motor and posture patterns and detailed videotracking-based analysis of swim path patterns are performed on all subjects.

The experimental tank was filled with mature fresh water that was aged and biologically filtered and had previously been exposed to zebrafish. The tank was illuminated from above by two 13-W fluorescent lightbulbs, and the test room was kept dark to obscure the external environment. Three sides of the tank were covered by a gray cardboard paper. The experimental fish was placed singly in the small experimental tank (21 � 12 � 24 cm), and after a 20-sec period, its behavior was recorded for 10 min. Upon completion of the recording, the fish was gently removed and placed in a small holding chamber.

Group preference. The zebrafish is a highly social species. It forms schools, a group of individuals that swim close to one another. Individual zebrafish are expected to be motivated to join a school. This preference for the group, also termed group cohesion, formed the basis of a behavioral test in which the effect of alcohol was investigated (Gerlai et al., 2000). The present test is a modification of this previously employed paradigm. After the novel open tank test, the experimental fish was removed from the test tank, was held in a small container for 1 min, and was placed back into the test tank. The partitions on the right and left sides of the experimental tank were removed to allow unobstructed view of two adjacent stimulus tanks. One of these tanks contained 10 stimulus fish, conspecific zebrafish of the same size and age as those of the test subject, and the other tank contained only fresh water but no stimulus fish. The positioning of the stimulus fish-that is, whether they were presented on the left or the right side of the experimental tank-was randomly balanced across experimental fish. Behavior was recorded for 10 min. At the end of the test, the experimental fish was gently removed and again placed into a small holding container until the next recording session (the aggression test) started.

Aggression test (the inclined mirror task). Solitary zebrafish encountering another individual often exhibit agonistic behavior, a response different in form and alcohol dose response characteristics from social behavior (Gerlai et al., 2000). Agonistic behaviors were tested following the group preference task. The partitions were returned to the sides of the experimental tank, and a mirror was placed behind the tank at a 22.5� angle to the back of the aquarium in such a way that the mirror image on the left side appeared closer and that on the right side appeared farther away. Since solitary fish of the same gender encountering each other often exhibit agonistic behaviors, rather than group cohesion, the "approaching" mirror image would be expected to elicit aggression. The behavior of the zebrafish was monitored in this test again for 10 min. The rationale for the positioning of the mirror was that it provided a lateral view of the "opponent," a sight that best elicits aggression (Gerlai et al., 2000). Furthermore, it allowed the experimental fish to view its "opponent" from closer or farther away while swimming along the longitudinal axis of the tank, which we expected would allow the experimenter to better quantify aggressive tendencies. Following the test, the experimental fish was again placed in a small container until the predator model test started.

Predator model test. The antipredatory behavior of the zebrafish is believed to be adaptive and, thus, likely to be under the influence of genetic factors (Csanyi, 1986; Gerlai, 1993). Furthermore, predator-model-elicited behavioral responses have been shown to be dependent on level of exposure to alcohol (Gerlai et al., 2000). These features suggest that predator-elicited responses are phenotypical characteristics that will allow the detection of mutational or pharmaceutical-agent-induced functional changes in the brain.

In the present study, a predator model similar in size and shape to that used before by Gerlai et al. (2000) was employed. The model was made of a 50-ml falcon tube that was filled with charcoal and water (and thus appeared black). The model had two plastic "eyes" (diameter, 8 mm; white "iris" and black "pupil") glued to the conical end of the tube. The right and left partitions were once again removed from the experimental tank, and the predator model was placed into the stimulus tank adjacent to the experimental tank and was moved using a transparent plastic rod attached to its back during the 1st and 10th minutes of the 10-min-long recording session. The positioning of the stimulus presentation was identical to the one used in the group preference task. That is, if the experimental fish was allowed to view a group of conspecifics on the right side of its experimental tank in the group preference test, for example, the predator model was also presented on that side.

Quantification of Behavior

Manual quantification. The method was similar to those published previously (Gerlai et al., 2000). The zebrafish were analyzed using manual quantification of the location and locomotor activity of the fish. Videotapes were replayed on a Sony (DVCAM, DSR-11) MiniDV digital cassette player connected to a 14-in. JVC TV monitor. A transparency with a grid pattern was placed on the monitor. Using The Observer software, the experimenter recorded the duration of time the fish spent in the upper or the lower half of the tank and on the left and the right sides of the tank. The time that the fish spent on the side opposite to the stimulus tank that contained the stimulus fish or the predator model and the time that the fish spent in the upper half of the tank were statistically analyzed. In addition, the total number of times that the fish entered the left, the right, the upper, and the lower halves of the tank was also analyzed, which served as a measure of general locomotor activity.

Videotracking. Videotracking was accomplished using the EthoVision Color Pro (Version 3.0) software (Noldus), an approach that was expected to allow quantification of swim path patterns, including the location and locomotor characteristics of the fish, more precisely than a manual recording method could, and without the need for the experimenter to view videotapes. The EthoVision software was configured to accept live input from a video camera fed directly into the computer. The signal from the camera was fed through a piccolo video card and was read by Etho Vision. Before each test, a background image was recorded of the empty experimental tank. After the subject had been placed in the tank, the program compared each incoming image sample with the original background. Image samples were taken at a rate of 10/sec. The software was configured to use a subtraction method of stimulus detection: The pixel values of each new sample image were subtracted from those of the background image, and discrepancies were detected. Detection threshold levels, the minimum difference between the values of 2 pixels accepted by the computer, were also set to minimize environmental noise (from water droplets, reflections, bubbles, etc.). Surface area was also recorded, defined as the number of adjacent pixels with differences above noise threshold. The pixel cluster with the largest surface area was interpreted as an "object," corresponding to the experimental subject, and the x-, y-coordinates of the center of that object were recorded. If no object of at least 25 pixels was located, the program recorded the coordinates of the last known location of the object (fewer than 5% of the samples). Tracks were recorded for the full 10 min of the test period. After recording was complete, the tracks were visually inspected for artifacts, and these were removed manually. This correction procedure was particularly important in the analysis of behavior in the aggression test, where the mirror image of the test fish was occasionally confused by the software with the actual subject (for further details and implications, see the Results and Discussion sections).

The following parameters were quantified.

1. Mean distance from bottom. The distance of the experimental fish from the bottom of the tank was measured every 0.10 sec, and the average distance was calculated for the 10-min recording session in each task. This measure was chosen because previous observations (e.g., Gerlai et al., 2000) had suggested that proximity of zebrafish to the bottom of the tank may represent a good measure of fear versus habituated state.

2. Mean distance from stimulus. The distance of the experimental fish from the glass wall of its test tank adjacent to the stimulus tank was recorded every 0.10 sec, and the mean of these distance values was calculated for the entire session length. Note that the stimulus (the group of conspecifics or the predator model) was presented at the same side for a given experimental fish but that the side changed randomly among experimental fish. Also note that in the novel open tank, no stimulus was presented on either side of the tank and that the side from which distance was quantified was chosen so as to be the same as the one in which the group of stimulus fish or the predator model would be presented. This distance measure was chosen because it allowed us to analyze social cohesion, aggression, and the effect of the predator model.

3. Total distance moved. To quantify locomotor activity, the total distance moved by the experimental fish was recorded. Quantification of all the distance measures was conducted after calibration of EthoVision by inputting the actual dimensions of the test tank. The distance measures are expressed in centimeters.

In addition to these measures, we also quantified the mean heading direction and the mean turning angle. Mean heading, a measure of direction of movement, is defined as the angle of movement, relative to the vertical line. The subject's location was measured every 0.10 sec, and a vector was calculated between that location (n) and the most recent point (n- 1). The average angle of this vector, relative to the vertical reference line, was taken for the entire session. Thus, 0� means movement straight upward, 180� means straight downward, 270� is horizontally toward the stimulus, and 90� means movement in the opposite direction (i.e., away from the stimulus). Turn angle is a measure of a tendency to change direction of movement, most prevalent in erratic (zig-zagging) or thrashing behaviors. Turn angle is calculated as the difference between two consecutive heading calculations, taken every 0.10 sec and averaged across the session.

Event recording. The behavioral measures described above for our videotracking analysis may not quantify motor and posture patterns. Usually, observation-based event recording is conducted for such purpose. The latter is based on one of the fundamental tenets of ethology, which postulates that an apparently continuous stream of behavior can be broken down into mutually exclusive distinct successive motor patterns that represent species-specific units of behavior (Huntingford, 1984). Indeed, we have successfully employed this approach in different species, including fish (e.g., Gerlai, Crusio, & Csanyi, 1990) and rodents (Gerlai et al., 1993). The Ethogram-that is, a complete list of species-specific motor and posture patterns-is not yet established for the zebrafish. Here, we recorded and quantified only six basic simple motor patterns that could be easily recognized and distinguished using The Observer event-recording software (Noldus). We acknowledge that, potentially, there are a large number of motor and posture patterns of the zebrafish that one may be able to define, differentiate, and quantify, but we also argue that the six behavioral units we recorded here are sufficient for our proof-of-concept analysis.

The following behavioral units (motor and posture patterns) were recorded: swimming (continuous locomotion with the use of the pectoral and caudal fins), thrashing (forceful back-and-forth swimming against the glass wall of the fish tank), floating (fish is stationary or is moving very slowly without using its caudal fin; pectoral, dorsal, and anal fins may open and close, or beat, with a low and stable frequency [no more than 1 beat/sec]), freezing (a motionless state during which only the gills and, occasionally, the eyes may move, which occurs mostly while the fish is on the bottom, in a corner, or right below the water surface), erratic movement (fast [more than 3 cm/sec swim speed] and seemingly aimless zig-zagging with frequent changes of the direction of swimming, which, often, occurs in the bottom of the tank but can be seen in midwater as well), and creeping (slow [less than 1 cm/sec speed] movement during which the caudal dorsal and anal fins are motionless and only the pectoral fins beat, most often observed after freezing and/or erratic movement). The duration, relative to session or interval length (%), was calculated for all the behavioral units.

Statistical Analysis

The analysis of the data was conducted using SPSS (Version 12.0.1 for the PC). Behavior of fish across multiple test situations was analyzed using a repeated measures ANOVA. In case of significant results, differences across test situations were further analyzed using the post hoc Tukey honestly significant difference (HSD) test. To investigate potential correlations among swim path parameters and motor and posture patterns, bivariate Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated, and the correlation matrices were subjected to a principal component analysis (PCA). The component matrices were subjected to Varimax rotation with Kaiser normalization. Retention of components was set at the minimum eigenvalue of 1.

RESULTS

The zebrafish exhibited different behavioral responses to the four test situations. These differences were detected similarly by the manual recording of the location and activity of the fish and by computerized videotracking. The results from the manual quantification of the location of the fish are shown in Figure 1A. According to these results, the fish spent about 50% of their time in the upper half of the tank in the novel open tank and in the aggression task, whereas the fish spent more than 60% of their time in the upper half of the tank in the group preference and the predator model tasks, a significant difference among tests [test situation, F(3,351) = 17.89, p < .001]. A post hoc Tukey HSD multiple comparison confirmed the results and showed that the fish spent significantly (p < .01) less time in the upper half of the tank in the novel open tank and in the aggression task than they did in the other test situations. The pattern of results obtained with this method is highly comparable to that obtained with the use of videotracking. Figure 1B shows the results for the behavioral measure distance from bottom, quantified using the videotracking software. This measure is obtained by recording the distance of the fish from the bottom of the tank (in centimeters) every 0.10 sec and obtaining the average of the distance values for the entire session (mean distance from bottom). The results show again that in the novel tank and in the aggression task, the fish stayed closer to the bottom (smaller values), whereas they stayed closer to the surface in the other two tasks. An ANOVA supported these observations [F(3,351) = 23.07, p < .001], and a Tukey HSD test also showed that in the open tank and aggression task, the fish were closer to the bottom than they were in the other two tasks (p < .01). The primary goal of the present study was to compare different behavioral quantification methods and to determine whether these methods could detect environmentally induced test-specific behavioral differences similarly. The differences in behavioral responses to the four test environments have been discussed before (Gerlai et al., 2000). Thus, here, we will only briefly state that the time spent near the surface and the differences in this measure are likely species-specific characteristics of zebrafish and are not due to biased stimulus positioning. For example, the increased time spent near the surface in the group preference task was not the result of the fact that the stimulus fish were near the surface. These fish were presented in a small stimulus tank, and their distribution in the tank was fairly homogeneous. Similarly, the predator model was also presented in the middle layers of the water.

Figure 2 shows the results that reflect the distance of the experimental fish from the side of its tank adjacent to the stimulus presented-that is, the side where the group of conspecifics, the predator model, or the closest view of the mirror image in the aggression test was. In the case of manual recording (Figure 2A), the experimenter quantified the amount of time the fish spent in the half of the tank opposite to the stimulus presentation side. An ANOVA showed significant differences across test situations [F(3,351) = 199.92, p < .001]. Not surprisingly, the fish in the novel open tank (no specific stimulus presented on either side) spent 50% of their time in each half of the tank. Presentation of a group of conspecifics dramatically reduced the time spent in the opposite half of the tank; that is, the experimental fish moved closer to the group of stimulus fish (Tukey HSD, p < .01). The analysis of response to the mirror image (aggression task) showed that the experimental fish had no side preference (Tukey HSD, p > .05, in comparison with performance in the novel open tank). This was somewhat surprising, since previously (Gerlai et al., 2000), we had observed a robust preference of the test fish to stay close to its mirror image, a discrepancy with respect to the present result that may have been due to differences in our present experimental setup (a smaller tank, leading to a smaller distance change between the test subject and its mirror image from one side of the tank to the other). Last, it is notable that the fish in the predator model task spent significantly more time in the opposite side of the tank-that is, away from the predator model (Tukey HSD, p < .05, in comparison with all the other test situations). Importantly, the pattern of results above is closely replicated by the computerized videotracking analysis. Here, the actual distance from the stimulus was quantified precisely. An ANOVA again showed a significant test situation effect [F(3,35l) = 568.94, p < .001], and a post hoc Tukey HSD test confirmed that the distance of the experimental fish from the stimulus was smallest in the group preference task (p < .01, in comparison with all the other tests) and largest in the predator model test (p < .01, in comparison with all the other tests).

Figure 3 shows the results reflecting the locomotor activity of the zebrafish in the four test paradigms, quantified using two methods: the manual recording (Figure 3A) and the videotracking technique (Figure 3B). The two methods employed were manual recording of shuttling activity among the four quadrants of the tank and videotracking, which measured total distance moved. These measures were chosen for the purpose of comparison, because shuttling activity is often used as a crude measure of locomotor activity in numerous species, ineluding zebrafish (see, e.g., Gerlai et al., 2000), but the most precise way to quantify amount of locomotion is to measure the actual distance moved, using videotracking. Thus, our question was whether the labor-intensive and approximate manually recorded measure would correlate with the precise computerized quantification parameter. Locomotor activity, measured as the total number of transitions among the four quadrants (upper left, upper right, lower left, and lower right) of the test tank (shuttling activity), showed significant differences from test situation to test situation [F(3,351) = 42.72, p < .001], with activity being highest in the novel open tank (Tukey HSD, p < .05, in comparison with all the other test situations) and lowest in the group preference task (Tukey HSD, p< .01, in comparison with all the other test situations). The pattern of results obtained with videotracking was fairly similar to this, with one exception: The actual total distance moved, measured in centimeters, was much higher in the group preference task than would have been predicted on the basis of the results obtained with the manual recording method. Although an ANOVA detected significant differences among test situations [F(3,351) = 22.23, p < .001], as did the manual-recording findings, a post hoc Tukey HSD test did not show the activity level of the fish in the group preference task to be different from that of all the other groups; in fact, activity in this task was significantly (p < .05) different (smaller) only when compared with the novel open field activity level. This discrepant finding was due to the fact that the fish in the group preference task spent most of their time swimming in the half of the tank closer to the stimulus fish and, thus, performed less shuttling activity between the left and the right sides of the tank. This resulted in a reduced value's being recorded by the manual method. But given that these fish were actively moving, trying to join their schoolmates in the other tank (see below), the videotracking system did not detect, and correctly so, such a dramatic reduction of locomotor activity as that found with the manual-recording method.

Figure 4 shows the results for mean heading direction, quantified using the videotracking software. No significant differences were found among test situations [F(3,351) = 0.91, p > .40]; that is, all values were around 180�, demonstrating that, on average, the fish swam in all directions in each task. Figures 4B and 4C show heading direction during the first and last (10th) minutes of the session in each task. The results suggest that heading direction will be a useful measure for more refined data analysis aimed at shorter time intervals preceding or following the presentation of particular stimuli. For example, in our analysis, an ANOVA revealed a significant test difference for both the first [F(3,351) = 21.27, p < .001] and the last [F(3,351) = 23.13, p < .001] minutes of the session, a finding that was due to the significant reduction of heading direction values in the predator test, in comparison with the other situations, during the first and the last minutes of the test (Tukey HSD, p < .01), demonstrating that during the presentation of the predator model (1st and 10th min), the zebrafish tended to head away from the stimulus presentation side.

The mean turning angle (Figure 5), reflecting the angular change in swimming direction, was largest in the two paradigms associated with social interaction-that is, the group preference and the aggression tasks. The differences were significant [ANOVA test effect, F(3,351) = 111.61, p < .001; Tukey HSD test: group preference and aggression test values differed from those for the other two tasks at p < .01, and no other differences were significant at p = .05]. According to our personal observations, the increased turning angle may reflect the intense thrashing (swimming against the glass-i.e., toward the group of conspecifics in the group preference task; see Figure 7C) and the aggressive display dance (not quantified) often observable in the aggression task.

In addition to the manual and videotracking-based analysis of the location and activity parameters of zebrafish behavior, we were also interested in the quantification of motor and posture patterns. These patterns are characteristic of the movements of zebrafish and may reflect unique features not captured by the traditional activity parameters. The Ethogram-that is, the list of characteristic species-specific motor and posture patterns-of zebrafish is not established yet. Here, we used only six basic motor and posture patterns for the sake of addressing the principal question: Are motor/posture patterns (recorded by event recording) and the activity parameters (recorded by videotracking) redundant measures of behavior?

Figure 6 shows the results of quantification of swimming. This active locomotory response is differentiated from another active swim pattern termed thrashing (see Figure 7). Swimming differs in form and, perhaps, even in what behavioral state it represents, from thrashing. Swimming is a more relaxed locomotion, whereas thrashing is more forceful, and the latter is always directed toward the glass wall (i.e., the fish swims back and forth, with its head pushed against the glass). The analysis of swimming revealed a significant effect of test situation [F(3,351) = 137.27, p < .001]. A post hoc Tukey HSD test showed that the value of swimming was highest in the predator model test, which differed from the second highest value obtained in the aggression test (p < .01), and the latter also differed significantly (p < .01) from the group preference and novel open tank values.

The fish performed significantly different amounts of thrashing in the four test paradigms [Figure 7A; F(3,351 ) = 120.92, p < .001 ], and the pattern of differences for thrashing was highly different from that for swimming. Thrashing (Figure 7A) was highest in the group preference paradigm and lowest in the predator model test (Tukey HSD, p < .05; all the groups differed from each other). When we define thrashing, we refer to the form-that is, the appearance-of the behavior. However, we acknowledge that this motor pattern may represent different aspects of zebrafish behavior, depending on the direction of thrashing-that is, whether it is performed toward or away from the stimulus presented. These two possibilities were distinguished in the analysis and are presented in Figures 7B and 7C. An analysis of thrashing on all the glass walls but the one adjacent to the stimulus showed significant test-situation-dependent differences [F(3,351) = 209.84, p < .001]. However, here, the value was smallest in the group preference task (Tukey HSD, p < .05, in comparison with all the other test situations). Similarly, the thrashing shown in Figure 7B is smaller, and not larger (as is shown in Figure 7A), in the aggression task, in comparison with the value obtained in the predator model task (Tukey HSD, p < .05). Clearly, the differences between the values shown in panels A and B of Figure 7 were due to the fact that the fish performed thrashing toward or away from the stimulus in a task-dependent manner (Figure 7C). The analysis of thrashing toward the stimulus (Figure 3C) showed a significant [F(3,351) = 220.25, p < .001] test situation effect, and a Tukey HSD test confirmed that the fish performed significantly differently in each test situation, with the largest values in the group preference task and the second largest in the aggression test. That is, a large proportion of the thrashing in the group preference task, and also in the aggression task, was directed toward the stimulus, since the experimental fish were attempting to get closer to their school mates or their opponents. In the novel open tank, no specific external stimulus was presented, and thus no thrashing toward the stimulus was recorded. Last, in the predator model task, thrashing was rarely performed by the test fish toward the stimulus side, since the experimental fish tended to avoid this side. However, given that the predator model was presented only for the first and the last minutes of the 10-min session, some thrashing toward the stimulus side did occur.

Figure 8 depicts motor and posture patterns that occurred rarely or for only short periods of time. These include erratic movement (Figure 8A), floating (Figure 8B), creeping (Figure 8C), and freezing (Figure 8D). Despite the low occurrence and, thus, the relatively higher variability, in comparison with the mean, these behaviors also showed test-dependent significant differences [erratic movement, F(3,351) = 30.24, p < .001; creeping, F(3,351) = 7.67, p < .001; floating, F(3,351) = 3.45, p < .05; freezing, F(3,351) = 3.41, p < .05]. Tukey HSD post hoc analyses showed a significantly (p < .05) higher erratic movement in the novel open tank than in all the other tests, a significantly elevated (p < .05) amount of creeping in the predator model task, in comparison with all the other tests, and significant ( p < .05) differences in floating and freezing between the novel open tank and the predator model test.

The means obtained with videotracking and event recording show an apparently different pattern across tests. That is, unlike in the case of videotracking and manual recording, generally no correlation is evident. Nevertheless, it is possible that behavioral measures obtained with videotracking and event recording covary at the interindividual level. To address this question, we analyzed bivariate Pearson correlation coefficients. Given the number of variables and test situations, the correlation matrix obtained was large (it contained [n � (n-1)]/2 = 946 bivariate correlation coefficients, where n = [5 video-tracking measures + 6 event-recording measures] * 4 test situations). To reduce this complexity, we subjected the correlation matrix to a multivariate statistical procedure, the PCA. Briefly, this procedure allows one to group behavioral measures that correlate with each other. A correlation group of behaviors is represented by a principal component, and behaviors belonging to such a group are listed under a component (also often called a factor) with large (.30 or larger) loadings, the correlation coefficient between the behavioral measure and the component. In addition, the PCA also addresses the question of how the same behavioral measures recorded in different tests correlate. For example, it is not obvious that swimming in the group preference task necessarily represents the same behavior as swimming, say, in the predator model test.

Table 1 shows the principal component loading structure for all behavioral measures recorded by event recording and videotracking in all four test situations. Thirteen principal components were obtained with greater than 1 eigenvalues, and these components are thus retained. These components explained more than 75% of the total variance, a reasonably large value. It must also be noted that given the rotation procedure (Varimax), the extracted principal components are orthogonal; that is, the correlation between them is zero. Principal Component 1 contains large loadings of motor/posture patterns recorded with event recording exclusively. It represents swimming and thrashing, with opposite signs recorded in all four test situations. Videotracking quantified behavioral measures are not represented in this component. Principal Component 2 is also exclusively of motor and posture patterns, and no videotracking measures have large loadings on this factor. It represents freezing in all four tests. Principal Component 3 has videotracking measures exclusively, and it mainly represents total distance moved in all four test situations and mean distance from the stimulus in two situations. Principal Component 4 again is of videotracking measures exclusively, and it is made up of large loadings of the measure distance from bottom. Principal Component 5 is the first mixed factor in which both videotracking and motor/posture patterns are present with large loadings. This component reflects the strength of social cohesion. Principal Component 6 has videotracking measures exclusively: turn angle across all situations. Conversely, Principal Component 7 has motor/ posture patterns exclusively, mainly floating across all four situations. Principal Component 8 has only three large loadings, the two largest being for creeping in the novel open tank and in the group preference test and a smaller one being for floating in the novel open tank. Principal Component 9 is made up of erratic movement in all four tests. Principal Component 10 is a mixed factor, but mainly characterizes swimming versus thrashing in the novel open tank. Principal Component 11 is made up of creeping in two tests. Principal Component 12 is a mixed factor whose composition is difficult to interpret. Finally, Principal Component 13 represents heading in three test situations.

In summary, the pattern of loadings above (Table 1) shows that a given principal component usually represents either videotracking or event-recording parameters, but rarely both; that is, behavioral measures quantified by videotracking or by event recording generally do not correlate with each other, a finding that is in line with the results shown in Figures 1-8. It is also interesting to note that some principal components have large loadings of the same behavioral measure recorded in multiple test situations, which implies that some common features or factors affect behavior similarly among multiple test paradigms.

DISCUSSION

The behavioral paradigms used in this article were previously developed with simplicity and automation in mind (Gerlai et al., 2000), but quantification of behavior was conducted using only manual recording of the location and activity of the fish. This time-consuming and labor-intensive method is inappropriate for high-throughput mutation screening, the ultimate goal behind the development of the paradigms. Here, a computerized videotracking-based quantification of swim paths, as well as a computer-aided observation-based analysis of motor patterns, event recording, was conducted, along with the manual recording of activity and swim location.

A comparison of the results of manual activity recording and videotracking suggests that videotracking could appropriately quantify the activity, as well as the location, of zebrafish. Thus, we conclude that manual recording can be replaced with this automated computerized method. Furthermore, unlike manual recording, videotracking could measure the precise location of the fish and could record numerous characteristics of their swim path, including speed, turning angle, heading direction, and so forth, which could not be quantified previously, using the manual method. With its better precision and the larger number of swim path characteristics it could quantify, videotracking is expected to better detect differences between experimental groups.

A drawback we noted with regard to videotracking concerns the experimental setup. For example, the human observer could easily recognize the experimental fish and tell it apart from its mirror image in the aggression task, whereas the videotracking system had a hard time differentiating the two. Thus, albeit sophisticated, videotracking is not foolproof. The test setup must be chosen carefully to avoid tracking errors. Numerous recommendations may be made in this regard. First, reflections must be avoided. For example, we now place the mirror in the aggression task on the side of the tank, and we use matte plastic sheets covering the otherwise reflective glass bottom and back side of the test tank. Second, lighting conditions must be optimal; for example, homogeneous illumination of the fish and the background must be achieved. Third, removal of visual disturbances-for example, floating debris or bubbles-is important. To achieve this, the water must be filtered, and nonpressurized water that has set for at least 24 h must be used (the latter prohibits bubbles from forming as a result of compressed gases coming out of solution in the water). Last, careful attention must be paid to the settings of Etho Vision-for example, specification of the minimum and maximum sizes of the target subject and adjustment of contrast levels.

Another result that emerged from the comparison of behavioral quantification methods employed in the present study is that the videotracking measures did not correlate with the event-recording measures. The latter motor patterns showed paradigm-dependent changes different from those measures obtained with videotracking. This observation was confirmed by a multivariate method, PCA. This result may seem surprising at first. However, one must note that motor patterns, as recorded here, represent a qualitative description of behavior and do not reflect intensity of behavior as much as videotracking measures do. For example, a fish can swim faster or slower, and thus, the recorded duration of time spent swimming (event recording) may not properly reflect the actual amount of locomotion (the length of swim path recorded by videotracking). Clearly, videotracking was superior to event recording in this regard. However, the videotracking measures employed here did not differentiate finer motor patterns as well as the human observer could with event recording. Thus, the two methods were complementary to each other. Briefly, this implies that the "standard" videotracking measures will miss some aspects of behavior and may not capture potential mutation-induced changes, the ultimate goal of the present study. It is expected that programming changes to be implemented for videotracking in the future will enable us to record numerous motor patterns without having to use the labor-intensive and slow observation-based event-recording method and that this will further increase the sophistication of automated behavior quantification.

Some motor and posture patterns may be easy to record using videotracking. For example, thrashing is characterized by a stereotypical pattern of swimming back and forth on or near the glass wall, and thus, this behavior may be quantified by EthoVision if one defines the area within which the behavior occurs (within 2 cm from the glass) and the swim pattern (e.g., no more than a 10-cm-long swim in one direction and/or more than 90� angular swim change within less then 2 sec). Similarly, several other motor and posture patterns could be recorded by EthoVision, including erratic movement (defined by the high speed of swimming and the frequent swim direction changes), leaping (defined by a single fast bout of swimming), freezing (no movement), and so forth.

Other motor or posture patterns may be more difficult to quantify. Nevertheless, we are planning to conduct a systematic analysis of swim path and posture patterns in order to identify characteristic swim trajectories corresponding to particular motor or posture patterns, and after the identification of such trajectories, we will program the videotracking system. The first step in this analysis will be the establishment of a detailed Ethogram of the zebra-fish. Once a large number of motor and posture patterns have been descriptively defined, the exact time periods within which a particular motor or posture pattern occurs during the recording will be identified, and the swim path patterns corresponding to all these periods will be analyzed. The library of characteristic swim path patterns common to the identified periods corresponding to a particular motor or posture pattern will allow us to properly program EthoVision to automatically detect and quantify these patterns. A conceptually similar idea has been proposed for the analysis of force-transducer-recorded activity parameters in mice (Fitch, Adams, Chancy, & Gerlai, 2002), and similar approaches using videotracking and computer-vision-based software learning algorithms are also being developed in the private sector (reviewed in Gerlai, 2002). Currently, the key problem in this analysis is the synchronization of time for the manually recorded motor patterns and the videotracking-system-recorded swim paths. This seemingly simple problem requires serious attention because the softwares employed in this study, although compatible with each other, do not record time similarly. Whereas the EthoVision videotracking system measures the precise time, Observer suffers from a problem, as follows. The human experimenter is expected to press a key corresponding to a behavior when the behavior starts. However, to achieve high precision, the experimenter needs to know in advance what behavior is going to start. This, of course, is not possible, so the result is a significant lag, a period during which the experimenter recognizes what has started and then presses the appropriate key. As a result of this lag, and because the length of this lag depends on reaction time and many other subjective, experimenter-dependent factors, precise synchronization of time at the resolution of seconds or better is very cumbersome, at least as far as the Noldus Observer and EthoVision programs are concerned.

In summary, our present results demonstrate that automated behavior quantification using videotracking is feasible with adult zebrafish, since this method properly quantified the differences observed in different behavioral paradigms. Importantly, this means that scaling up the tests-and thus, higher throughput-is feasible. The simplicity of these behavioral tests and their previously shown ability to detect acute alcohol-treatment-induced behavioral alterations (Gerlai et al., 2000), combined with the present findings demonstrating the feasibility of automated behavior quantification, suggest that the zebrafish may be utilized in forward genetic or pharmacological analysis of alcohol effects. Despite its good potential, however, one must also acknowledge that, although promising, zebrafish behavior as an emerging line of research is still in its infancy. A lot of fundamental behavioral characterization studies are needed before the zebrafish will be regarded as a "mainstream" model organism of behavioral neuroscience and behavioral genetics.

[Reference]

REFERENCES

BAIER, H., KLOSTERMANN, S., TROWE, T., KARLSTROM, R. O., NUSSLEIN-VOLHARD, C., & BONHOEFFER, F. (1996). Genetic dissection of the retinotectal projection. Development, 123, 415-425.

BANG, P. I., YELICK, P. C., MALICKI, J. J., & SEWELL, W. F. (2002). High-throughput behavioral screening method for detecting auditory response defects in zebrafish. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 118, 177-187.

BARBAZUK, W. B., KORF, I., KADAVI, C., HEYEN, J., TATE, S., WUN, E., ET AL. (2000). The syntenic relationship of the zebrafish and human genomes. Genome Research, 10, 1351-1358.

BROCKERHOFF, S. E., HURLEY, J. B., JANSSEN-BIENHOLD, U., NEUHAUSS, S. C. F, DRIEVER, W., & DOWLING, J. E. (1995). A behavioral screen for isolating zebrafish mutants with visual system defects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92, 10545-10549.

BYERS, D., DAVIS, R. L., & KIGER, J. A., JR. (1981). Defect in cyclic AMP phosphodiesterase due to the dunce mutation of learning in Dmsophila melanogaster. Nature, 289, 79-81.

CANGER, A. K., PASSINI, M. A., ASCH, W. S., LEAKE, D., ZAFONTE, B. T, GLASGOW, E., & SCHECHTER, N. (1998). Restricted expression of the neuronal intermediate filament protein plasticin during zebrafish development. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 399, 561-572.

CARVAN, M. J., III, LOUCKS E., WEBER, D. N., & WILLIAMS, F. E. (2004). Ethanol effects on the developing zebrafish: Neurobehavior and skeletal morphogenesis. Neurotoxicology & Teratology, 26, 757-768.

CERDA, J., CONRAD, M., MARKL, J., BRAND, M., & HERRMANN, H. (1998). Zebrafish vimentin: Molecular characterization, assembly properties and developmental expression. European Journal of Cell Biology, 77, 175-187.

CONCHA, M. L., & ADAMS, R. J. (1998). Oriented cell divisions and cellular morphogenesis in the zebrafish gastrula and neurula: A time-lapse analysis. Development, 125, 983-994.

CS�NYI, V. (1986). Ethological analysis of predator avoidance by the paradise fish (Macropodus operations L.): II. Key stimuli in avoidance learning. Animal Learning & Behavior, 14, 101-109.

CURRIE, P. D. (1996). Zebrafish genetics: Mutant cornucopia. Current Biology, 6, 1548-1552.

DARLAND, T., & DOWLING, J. E. (2001). Behavioral screening for cocaine sensitivity in mutagenized zebrafish. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 11691-11696.

DETRICH, W. H., III, WESTERFIELD, M., & ZON, L. I. (EDS.) (1999). The zebrafish: Vol. 1. Biology. San Diego: Academic Press.

DLUGOS, C. A., & RABIN, R. A. (2003). Ethanol effects on three strains of zebrafish: Model system for genetic investigations. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior, 74, 471-480.

DONOVAN, A., BROWNLIE, A., ZHOU, Y, SHEPARD, J., PRATT, S. J., MOYNIHAN, J., ET AL. (2000). Positional cloning of zebrafish ferroportinl identifies a conserved vertebrate iron exporter. Nature, 403, 776-781.

EISEN, J. S. ( 1991 ). Motoneuronal development in the embryonic zebrafish. Development, 1, 141-147.

EISEN, J. S. (1996). Zebrafish make a big splash. Cell, 87, 969-977.

FETCHO, J. R., & LIU K. S. (1998). Zebrafish as a model system for studying neuronal circuits and behavior. In O. Kiehn, R. M. Harris-Warwick, L. M. Jordan, H. Hultborn, & N. Kudo (Eds.), Neuronal mechanisms for generating locomotor activity (Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 860, pp. 333-345). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

FETCHO, J. R., & O'MALLEY, D. M. (1995). Visualization of active neural circuitry in the spinal cord of intact zebrafish. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73, 399-406.

FITCH, T., ADAMS, B., CHANEY, S., & GERLAI, R. (2002). Force transducer based movement detection in fear conditioning in mice: A comparative analysis. Hippocampus, 12, 4-17.

GEISLER, R., RAUCH, G. J., BAIER, H., VAN BEBBER, F., BROBETA, L., DEKENS, M. P., ET AL. ( 1999). A radiation hybrid map of the zebrafish genome. Nature Genetics, 23, 86-89.

GERLAI, R. (1993). Can paradise fish (Macropodus opercularis) recognize its natural predator? An ethological analysis. Ethology, 94, 127-136.

GERLAI, R. (2002). Phenomics: Fiction or the future? Trends in Neurosciences, 25, 506-509.

GERLAI, R. (2003). Zebrafish: An uncharted behavior genetic model. Behavior Genetics, 33,461-468.

GERLAI, R., & CLAYTON, N. S. (1999). Analysing hippocampal function in transgenic mice: An ethological perspective. Trends in Neurosciences, 22,47-51.

GERLAI, R., CRUSIO, W. E., & CS�NYI, V. (1990). Inheritance of species specific behaviors in the paradise fish (Macropodus opercularis): A diallel study. Behavior Genetics, 20,487-498.

GERLAI, R., FRIEND, W., BECKER, L., O'HANLON, D., MARKS, A., & RODER, J. (1993). Female transgenic mice carrying multiple copies of the human gene for S100 beta are hyperactive. Behavioural Brain Research, 55, 51-59.

GERLAI, R., LAHAV, M., GUO, S., & ROSENTHAL, A. (2000). Drinks like a fish: Zebrafish (Danio rerio) as a behavior genetic model to study alcohol effects. Pharmacology Biochemistry & Behavior, 67, 773-782.

GRANATO, M., & NUSSLEIN-VOLHARD, C. ( 1996). Fishing for genes controlling development. Current Opinion in Genetics & Development, 6,461-468.

GRANATO, M., VAN EEDEN, F. J. M., SCHACH, U., TROWE, T., BRAND, M., FURUTAMI-SEIKI, M., ET AL. ( 1996). Genes controlling and mediating locomotion behavior of the zebrafish embryo and larva. Development, 123, 399-413.

GRUNWALD, D. J. (1996). A fin-de-si�cle achievement: Charting new waters in vertebrate biology. Science, 274, 1634-1635.

GUO, S., YAMAGUCHI, Y, SCHILBACH, S., WADA, T., GODDARD, A., LEE, J., ET AL. (2000). A regulator of transcriptional elongation, which is required for vertebrate neuronal development. Nature, 408, 366-369.

HAFFTER, P., & NUSSLEIN-VOLHARD, C. (1996). Large-scale genetics in a small vertebrate, the zebrafish. International Journal of Developmental Biology, 40, 221-227.

HOLDER, N., & MCMAHON, A. (1996). Genes from zebrafish serenes. Nature, 384, 515-516.

HUKRIEDE, N. A., JOLY, L., TSANG, M., MILES, J., TELLIS, P., EPSTEIN, J. A., ET AL. (1999). Radiation hybrid mapping of the zebrafish genome. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96, 9745-9750.

HUNTINGFORD, F. (1984). The study of animal behaviour. London: Chapman & Hall.

KNAPIK, E. W., GOODMAN, A., EKKER, M., CHEVRETTE, M., DELGADO, J., NEUHAUSS, S. C. F., ET AL. (1998). A microsatellite genetic linkage map for zebrafish (Danio rerio). Nature Genetics, 18, 338-343.

KRATZ, E., DUGAS, J. C., & NGAI, J. (2002). Odorant receptor gene regulation: Implications from genomic organization. Trends in Genetics, 18, 29-34.

LEVIN, L. R., HAN, P.-L., HWANG, P. M., FEINSTEIN, P. G., DAVIS, R. L., & REED, R. R. (1992). The Drosophila learning and memory gene rutabaga encodes a Ca2+/Calmodulin-responsive adenylyl cyclase. Cell, 68, 479-489.

LIU, K. S., & FETCHO, J. R. (1999). Laser ablations reveal functional relationships of segmental hindbrain neurons in zebrafish. Neuron, 23, 325-335.

LORENT, K., LIU, K. S., FETCHO, J. R., & GRANATO, M. (2001). The zebrafish space cadet gene controls axonal pathfinding of neurons that modulate fast turning movements. Development, 128, 2131-2142.

LOUCKS, E., & CARVAN M.J., III (2004). Strain-dependent effects of developmental ethanol exposure in zebrafish. Neurotoxicology & Teratology, 26, 745-755.

MALICKI, J., SCHIER, A. F., SOLNICA-KREZEL, L., STEMPLE, D. L., NEUHAUSS, S. C., STAINIER, D. Y., ET AL. (1996). Mutations affecting development of the zebrafish ear. Development, 123, 275-283.

MCCALLUM, C. M., COMAI, L., GREENE, E. A., & HENIKOFF, S. (2000). Targeting induced local lesions in genomes (TILLING) for plant functional genomics. Plant Physiology, 123, 439-442.

NASEVICIUS, A., & EKKER, S. C. (2000). Effective targeted gene "knockdown" in zebrafish. Nature Genetics, 26, 216-220.

NEUHAUSS, S. C. F. (2003). Behavioral genetic approaches to visual system development and function in zebrafish. Journal of Neurobiology, 54, 148-160.

NEUHAUSS, S. C. F., BIEHLMAIER, O., SEELIGER, M. W., DAS, T., KOHLER, K., HARRIS, W. A., & BAIER, H. ( 1999). Genetic disorders of vision revealed by a behavioral screen of 400 essential loci in zebrafish. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 8603-8615.

SCHIER, A. F. ( 1997). Genetics of neural development in zebrafish. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 119-126.

SPRAGUE, J., DOERRY, E., DOUGLAS, S., & WESTERFIELD, M. (2001). The zebrafish information network (ZFIN): A resource for genetic, genomic and developmental research. Nucleic Acid Research, 29, 87-90.

STICKNEY, H. L., SCHMUTZ, J., WOODS, I. G., HOLTZER, C. C., DICKSON, M. C., KELLY, P. D., ET AL. (2002). Rapid mapping of zebrafish mutations with SNPs and oligonucleotide microarrays. Genome Research, 12, 1929-1934.

TROPEPE, V, & SIVE, H. L. (2003). Can zebrafish be used as a model to study the neurodevelopmental causes of autism? Genes, Brain & Behavior, 2, 268-281.

WESTERFIELD, M. (1993). The zebrafish book. Eugene: University of Oregon Press.

WIENHOLDS, E., SCHULTE-MERKER, S., WALDERICH, B., & PLASTERK, R. H. A. (2002). Target selected inactivation of the zebrafish ragl gene. Science, 297, 99-102.

WOODS, I. G., KELLY, P. D., CHU, F., NGO-HAZELETT, P., YAN, Y. L., HUANG, H., ET AL. (2000). A comparative map of the zebrafish genome. Genome Research, 10, 1903-1914.

ZHANG, J., TALBOT, W. S., & SCHIER, A. F. (1998). Positional cloning identifies zebrafish one-eyed pinhead as a permissive EGF-related ligand required during gastrulation. Cell, 92, 241-251.

ZHDANOVA, I. V., WANG, S. Y., LECLAIR, O. U., & DANILOVA, N. P. (2001). Melatonin promotes sleep-like state in zebrafish. Brain Research, 903, 263-268.

(Manuscript received September 6, 2005; revision accepted for publication November 11, 2005.)

[Author Affiliation]

RACHEL BLASER

University of Hawaii at Manoa, Honolulu, Hawaii

and

ROBERT GERLAI

University of Hawaii at Manoa, Honolulu, Hawaii and University of Toronto, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada

[Author Affiliation]

We thank Andromeda Axcell, Carrie Burns, Min Ku Kim, and Eva Leung for their technical help. We are also grateful to Robert Blanchard, Marilyn Dunlap, and Patricia Couvillon, who, in many ways, facilitated the completion of this study. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to R. Gerlai, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto, 3359 Mississauga Road, Mississauga, ON, L5L 1C6 Canada (e-mail: rgerlai@utm.utoronto.ca).